Home Psychology
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Critical, community and indigenous psychologiesTo complement cross-cultural and cultural psychology (see Chapters 3 and 4), another identifiable strand in research into cultural issues in the study of human behaviour and experience comes from critical, community and indigenous psychologies (Kagan et al., 2011; Burton & Kagan, 2015; Jahoda, 2016; Shwarz, 2018). If cultural psychology is distinct because of its ideas about the culture-mind relationship, critical, community and indigenous psychologies is distinguished by what might be termed an activist stance regarding the aims and nature of psychological enquiry. Critical, community-oriented researchers practice goal-directed research with the aim of transforming situations of inequality and oppression. Their outlook is at once theoretical and action-oriented, with the accent on both the practical (orientated to principled social change and liberation from oppression and disadvantage) and critical - questioning assumptions in dominant ideology and policy, in communities and their practice, and in psychology. (Burton & Kagan, 2015, p. 184) Meanwhile, indigenous psychology is a critical approach that is constructed from culturally diverse traditions in psychology. The indigenous psychology movement is a response to the historic hegemony of North American and European traditions, thus seeking to adapt the needs of psychology to diverse cultures (Jahoda, 2016). We will explore the indigenous psychology movement during the course of this chapter. First though, a review of critical and community approaches. Critical community psychology For critical and community psychology, research is a political act. It is a stepping stone towards the transformation of oppressive situations. Critical psychology is applied before it is theoretical. It aspires to develop evidence-based strategies for addressing inequality and liberating marginalised groups (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). Critical psychology originated in Latin America, following the work of pioneers such as Brazilian educator Paulo Freire (1921-1997), Colombian sociologist Orlando Fals Borda (1925-2008) and Spanish social psychologist Ignacio Martin- Baro (1942-1989) (see Figure 5.1). These radical thinkers developed their ideas and writings amid military conflicts and economic crises that ![]() Figure 5.1 Ignacio Martin-Baro, pioneer of Liberation Psychology affected their countries during the 1970s and 1980s (Sanchez, 1996). Indeed, Ignacio Martin-Baro was assassinated for his beliefs, along with many others, in El Salvador in 1989. Whilst rooted in Latin America during its nascent period, across several of the world’s less affluent regions critical psychology has contributed to programmes for addressing inequality, developing building projects for low-income groups, resettling victims of urban renewal and tackling violent crime (Sanchez, 1996). Its action-oriented approach displays a commitment amongst critical community psychologists to produce work that is oriented towards ‘amelioration of social ills and transformative action in relation to their causes’ (Burton & Kagan, 2015, p. 183). Conceptual cornerstones of critical psychology Psychologists who adhere to a critical approach do so in relation to four conceptual cornerstones; power, collective wellbeing, oppression and liberation (Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002).
Viewed in the context of power, collective wellbeing, oppression and liberation, we can see that for critical psychologists, all research has a political dimension. It is not a value-free enterprise. Like cultural psychology, the critical approach rejects the portrayal of researchers as objective observers (Nsamenang, 2000). Instead they are seen as active participants in (and interpreters of) their subject matter. Research is carried out by people with values and ideological standpoints (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). All humans are regarded as agents who have intentions and capacities to change the circumstances in which they live (Eckensberger, 1996; Burton & Kagan, 2015). This human agency casts us all in the role of goal-directed beings, not mere products of our circumstances and surroundings. Critical psychology in the community Building on the contributions of pioneers referred to in the previous section, recent researchers have sought to embed critical psychology in a more explicitly community-oriented context. This has yielded an approach that has become known as critical community psychology (Kagan et al., 2011; Burton & Kagan, 2015). Whilst adhering to the conceptual cornerstones of critical psychology, critical community psychology seeks to refine this position in relation to five principles; prefigurative action, diverse communities, exclusion, ecology, interdisciplinary methods (Burton & Kagan, 2015). These principles guide the practice of critical community psychology, illustrating how the conceptual cornerstones of critical psychology extend into communities.
Transformative research: psychology for social change Critical community psychology is founded on the principle that research can be positively transformative. The transformative potential of psychological knowledge is a sleeping giant that motivates critical research. Advocates of this paradigm want to awaken the rest of the discipline to the possibilities of research as an instrument of social change (Prilleltensky & Nelson, 2002). Transformative research is built on the conceptual cornerstones and guiding principles or the critical paradigm, outlined above. Instead of dwelling on people’s reactions to inequality and oppression, KEY TERM Transformative research. Research that investigates how disadvantaged individuals or groups can achieve social justice by bringing about change in their material and political circumstances. transformative research investigates how disadvantaged individuals or groups can achieve social justice by bringing about change in their material and political circumstances, either in the present or by paving the way for change prefiguratively (Burton & Kagan, 2015). Examples of transformative research questions include:
Critical psychology from the community: participatory action research (PAR) Critical psychology urges a shift in the research methods of global psychology. The proposed and, in many regions, already developing critical method acknowledges that wellbeing can only be understood by researchers who are prepared to look beyond the responses of individual participants, towards action in wider community and socio-political contexts (Sloan, 1996). A key strand of critical transformative research is represented by the emergence of participatory action research (PAR), which has been referred to as: A dynamic process of knowledge generation and sharing paired with action that happens within the frame of consciousness raising. (Kidd et al., 2018, p. 77) In adherence to the aforementioned guiding principles of critical psychology, PAR acknowledges inequalities in access to knowledge and power that exist in many societies. It also acknowledges the increasingly proactive nature of many participant groups who are keen to set their own research agendas and work with researchers on a consultative basis. As a strategy for promoting participant-researcher equality in the practice of carrying out research, PAR advocates a move towards the collective empowerment of those with whom research is carried out. Thus, PAR is carried out by researchers who position themselves on an equal power footing to participants who become partners in research. As part of this process, PAR is likely to be instigated by partner-participants with the aim of addressing a specific challenge in their community. The role of the researcher in this scenario is therefore to engage and work ![]() Figure 5.2 A cycle of PAR with community members to devise valuable means of collecting data and alleviating a challenge that has been identified by the community. Approaching the research scenario with humility, openness and flexibility, a reflexive cycle of PAR (see Figure 5.2) may be set in motion. This casts the researcher as an involved participant, whilst at the same time working with the community to generate data using a range of methods that have been selected for their appropriateness. PAR in practice in Nunavut, Arctic Canada An example of a PAR project comes from Krai (2012), who used this reflexive cycle to investigate suicide prevention with Inuit communities in Nunavut, Arctic Canada. Initially, at a suicide prevention event in Nunavut, indigenous community members suggested that an appropriate starting point of addressing high levels of suicide would be to ask local people what made them happy and what made them sad, why they thought suicide was so common and how it could be prevented. At the conference the indigenous community negotiated, in the presence of the researcher, the sole non-community member, a series of collaborative and culturally appropriate methods for gathering answers to the above questions. An Inuit steering group, consisting of elders, community workers and young people, was set up and charged with collecting data. The steering group, along with the researcher, designed interview questions and surveys, which were administered through local schools and com?munity groups, to participants aged between 14 and 94. The researcher participated as an interviewer, asking questions arising from steering groups suggestions. The outcome of the study saw the emergence of several themes relating to the importance of family, interpersonal communication and cultural values as factors that might alleviate suicide. These findings were disseminated to the community by the indigenous steering group, which led to the development of a local youth centre, counselling group and recreational facilities. Krai reports a decrease in suicide of 68% in the community, following the opening and reopening (after a brief period of closure) of the youth centre. Critical community psychology remains outside the global mainstream and, as the comments below show, does have its critics. Yet its orientation towards finding solutions in areas characterised by poverty, inequality and discrimination may mean that from its regional base in the developing world it will someday export its ideas and methods elsewhere (Moghaddam, 1990). After all, oppression, inequality and discrimination are not exclusive to nations in the Global South. Limitations of critical psychology
Indigenous psychologies: where is psychology? Psychology students traditionally begin their studies by asking - what is psychology? It may take the entire course to find a satisfactory answer, but as a rite of passage for students of human behaviour, tackling this question is more or less mandatory. But what about a second question, concerning psychology’s whereabouts? Where is psychology? This question is less common on mainstream psychology courses, yet we are duty-bound to ask it. After all, if psychology professes to be the study of all human behaviour and experience - wherever it may occur - its students really ought to be curious about where in the world most of its research actually takes place. The whereabouts of the written word in psychology Take a look at your psychology core textbook or the online resources for your course. Is most of the research featured there published in the US, with perhaps a smaller proportion from Europe? Is there much from Cameroon, India or Finland? Are your answers to these questions yes and no by any chance? It is likely that most of the books and articles featured in your textbook derive from research carried out by North American (and a smaller proportion of European) researchers, using North American (and a smaller proportion of European) participants. Psychology publishing reflects the accumulated knowledge and efforts of writers, researchers and participants who are unevenly distributed around the world. Moghaddam (1987b) once provocatively suggested that this unevenness reflects psychology’s own stratification into separate geographical regions, each with varying levels of influence over the discipline. He identified a tendency for mainstream psychology to gather and publish a disproportionate segment of data from selected geographical and cultural locations, mostly concentrated in North America (known as psychology’s ‘first world’) and to generalise these data worldwide. You could call this a sampling error (an error involving taking results from a restricted sample of participants and mistakenly applying them to the population as a whole) which jeopardises psychology’s claim to be the study of all human behaviour and experience, wherever it may occur. Moghaddam points out that traditionally psychology’s first world has claimed the greatest concentration of material resources for producing research data, leaving the discipline vulnerable to accusations of ethnocentric ‘western’ bias. So, is the production of psychology’s printed word (books and journal articles) overconcentrated in ‘the west’? Figure 5.3 does suggest so. The whereabouts of psychology's practitioners Your place of residence may influence your chances of becoming a professional psychologist. As far back as 1985 the US had 23 times as many academic psychologists working in universities as Britain had, and 234 times as many as Nigeria (Moghaddam, 1987b). But the global distribution of psychologists is in flux and not entirely predictable. In 2016 it was reported by the World Health Organization that Monaco (41), Norway (30) and Belgium (20) headed a league table showing the preponderance of psychiatrists per 100,000 of the population. At the other end of the scale India (0.3), Turkey (1.5) and Brazil (4.5) showed far fewer psychiatrists per 100,000 (World Health Organization, 2015). This predominance of psychology in the west is also reflected in psychological research publications (Bornmann et al., 2012). Surveying the incidence of publication and citation of academic articles internationally, their results ![]() Figure 5.3 Is there culture bias in psychology books and articles? indicate that the most-cited psychological research articles come from USA, UK, Germany, Netherlands, Ireland, Belgium, Sweden, Finland, Australia, Taiwan, and from English-language speaking countries elsewhere (Bornmann et al., 2012). Similarly, based on indicators such as average production of articles and citations, the US was shown to be the leading producer of psychological knowledge worldwide, followed by the UK and Canada, with these three hogging more than 60% of the total production (Garcia-Martinez et al., 2012). Yet the extent of the US hegemony may be in decline in relation to psychological personnel. It is likely that this US dominance will recede in coming decades (Stevens & Gielen, 2007). The early twenty-first century saw an upsurge in the number of psychologists across Spain, Israel, South Africa, Brazil and Argentina (Stevens & Wedding, 2004). In terms of the number of licensed psychologists, Buenos Aires is a world capital (Klappenbach, 2004). Clearly the US remains the world leader in terms of publishing, resources and personnel, but our discipline is now a global phenomenon. True, you are still more likely to learn about psychology from textbooks, online resources and journals that draw heavily on US research. But in terms of where in the world you are likely to practise psychology, opportunity appears to be knocking across five continents for both researchers and applied work. Indeed, as we will learn in the next section of this chapter, research interests of psychologists are becoming increasingly international and sensitive to indigenous questions about behaviour and experience. The indigenous psychology movement The indigenous psychology movement dates back to over half a century ago, where it began in the Philippines (Paredes-Canilao & Babaran- Diaz, 2013) and Taiwan (Hwang, 2005). Thereafter it expanded globally, reaching its peak of influence around the turn of the century (Jahoda, 2016). The very existence of the indigenous psychology movement reflects the critical idea that the dominance of western knowledge bases over psychological practises around the world represents a form of cultural imperialism (Jahoda, 2016). In response to this hegemony, the movement reflects a burgeoning global network of researchers and practitioners representing diverse regional traditions in psychological research. This illustrates the idea that psychology should reflect the realities and preoccupations of diverse cultures. Indeed, it has been argued that indigenous psychologies in India, Japan, Latin America and other regions represent an explicit revolt against North American dominance of the field (Pawlik & Rosenzweig, 2000; Allwood, 2005). Indigenous psychologies are a reminder that western psychology is neither sufficient nor appropriate for addressing psychological realities that are rooted in diverse cultures (Jahoda, 2016). Practitioners in the Global South lament mainstream psychology’s apparent indifference to psychological phenomena (poverty, illiteracy, civil war) that are especially pertinent to less affluent countries (Kwang-Kuo Hwang, 2017). Psychology’s ignorance of nonwestern philosophical traditions (Confucianism, Buddhism) is illustrated in this critique: Psychology as we knew it ... appeared to be the indigenous psychology of America or perhaps, more specifically, the psychology of middle-class Anglo-America. (Markus & Kitayama, 2003, p. 280) According to this view US research reflects US concerns, just as Ugandan research reflects Ugandan concerns, Australian Australian concerns and so on. The problem with this scenario is that while each of these indigenous psychologies represent distinct traditions, they have unequal influence worldwide. We have already seen that a disproportionate amount of resources and research emanates from the US, whose citizens’ behaviour and experiences are therefore more widely documented than are those of others. This imbalance exacerbates the need for us to explore a wider range of indigenous traditions in psychology, and to explore the idea of producing culture-inclusive psychological theories (Kwang-Kuo Hwang, 2017). One manifestation of this approach would be the rise of an indigenous psychology from Asia that stems from culturally relevant belief systems such as Buddhist, Confucian and Hindu religions and philosophies (Jahoda, 2016). Whatever the precise manifestation, we can assert here that the indigenous psychology movement is a response to an historical dominance of western cultural concerns in our discipline. This response reflects a psychology that has adapted to the needs of a particular cultures and countries with regionally diverse traditions of its own, which will now be illustrated with some examples. Indigenous psychology traditions from around world Indigenous researchers provide pictures from around the globe, painted by practitioners who have variously chosen to study, for example, human development in Cameroon (Nsamenang, 1992), psychopathology in New Guinea (Schieffelin, 1985), self-esteem in Brazil (Lane & Sawaia, 1991), African indigenous healing (Bojuwoye & Moletsane- Kekae, 2018), Islamic psychology (Haque, 2018). Indigenous psychology is more established and autonomous in some nations than it is in others. India is acknowledged to have the longest established research tradition among all nations in the Global South. India’s first psychology laboratory opened at the University of Calcutta in 1915. Sub-Saharan Africa was a later starter. The University of Nigeria opened the doors of the region’s first psychology department in 1963. In many countries, psychology was initially introduced by outsiders, though indigenous interests and specialisations later emerged. Pakistan and Bangladesh are examples of this. In East Asia too, psychology and psychiatry were introduced during the era of western colonisation from the nineteenth and twentieth centuries (Lee, 2018). Though psychology arrived in many regions with colonialists, ‘homegrown’ research interests have since followed distinctive paths. For example, Islamic scholars have written extensively on social and developmental psychology using ideas that integrate Islamic culture and belief systems (Haque, 2018). Bangladeshi researchers have investigated motivational issues surrounding agricultural development. Psychology in Pakistan has developed an interest in gender studies (Shouksmith, 1996). In East Asia, moves towards the indigenisation of psychology reflects a response to the dominance of western medicine in the treatment of maladies such as distress, weakness, fatigue and depression. Here, practitioners increasingly seek to incorporate Confucianism into their practice (Lee, 2018). India’s indigenous traditions in psychology is reflected in a growth in applied clinical psychology in its urban centres, as a corollary of increasing numbers of professional psychologists across the country (Shouksmith, 1996). Additionally, Hindu philosophies are now making their way into western healing contexts with increasing regularity (Rodrigues, 2018). Similarly, indigenous models of healing are increasingly being developed across Australasia (Waitoki et al., 2018). Within the indigenous psychology movement, research questions have tended to relate closely to cultural circumstances and political realities. Thus, in Bangladesh in the 1960s, since rural development was a pressing concern, psychologists were involved in studying motivational aspects of agricultural practice. In Thailand in the 1950s, where the development of educational programmes was especially high on the agenda, psychologists ploughed their energies into the applied researching of child development (Shouksmith, 1996). In Costa Rica in the 1980s, where neighbouring El Salvador and Nicaragua were living through revolutionary upheavals, war and its psychological effects were a high priority (Dobles Oropeza, 2000). It is worth remembering psychology is a relatively young phenomenon in many countries, yet we can see a growth in indigenous research interests and an increasing diversity of global healing traditions (Fernando & Moodley, 2018). Such indigenisation in response to local circumstances is a recurring theme (Sinha, 1997; Jahoda, 2016). Enriquez (1993) has argued that the indigenisation process can be instigated from either ‘without’ or ‘within’. In the former, research questions and methods are imported into a region by outsider researchers before being modified in response to local concerns. Conversely, indigenisation from within involves the formulation of unique research questions in response to a cultural group’s norms, priorities and everyday realities. Chinese research into behavioural aspects of Confucianism’s teachings (perseverance, thrift) is an example of this (Chinese Culture Connection, 1987; Lee, 2018). By extension, these indigenous psychological concepts can, in an era of increased globalisation, then be exported to the west, as in the case of many indigenous healing traditions (Rodrigues, 2018). Table 5.1 outlines in more details some of the key ideas to have emerged from indigenous psychologies from India, New Zealand and El Salvador, and from an Islamic perspective. TABLE 5.1 Indigenous psychologies from around the world
KEY TERM Indigenous psychologies. Diverse regional traditions in psychological research, reflecting differing cultural concerns. Indigenous methods The distinctiveness of indigenous psychology goes beyond simply devising questions that have particular relevance. It extends to finding regionally relevant methods for investigating these questions. For example, a characteristically European methodological preference for naturalistic, qualitative data collection emerged out of the social representation approach to psychology (Moscovici, 1976). Though not used by all European psychologists, such methods are more common there than, say, in the US. The approach stresses the importance of communally shared aspects of identity, and often translates into research that asks how we define ourselves as members of some groups and not others (Catholics or Protestants, male or female). European traditions show less interest here in studying human beings as individual units. Instead, the relationships between people are high on the research agenda. This difference in emphasis has spawned a European indigenous methodological preference for contextualised (naturalistic), interview-based research, rather than quasi-experimental methods using artificial scenarios and high levels of control (Smith et al„ 2006a). Indigenous psychology can complement the mainstream While indigenous psychologies are often seen as a challenge to mainstream cross-cultural psychology, the two approaches may be more compatible than they appear at a glance. Diverse traditions from across the globe can complement the work of mainstream psychologists who try to uncover culturally universal phenomena (Allwood, 2005). It has been suggested that global traditions in psychology will ultimately combine towards a culturally universal discipline, although this may seem a utopian idea (Jahoda, 2016). More realistically, we can expect cross-cultural research to adapt its methods from indigenous traditions and to gain expertise from working in different settings. Very often this kind of knowledge is provided by so-called bicul- tural researchers: practitioners who are indigenous to the region, but who trained elsewhere before returning home to practise (Stevens & Gielen, 2007; Jahoda, 2016). Indigenous and cross-cultural research can form mutually beneficial partnerships that draw on the unique contributions of researchers from different regions. Cross-cultural research may offer a degree of objectivity that is encapsulated in third-person accounts as they test their theories in ‘other cultures’. Indigenous research provides local knowledge and diverse accounts that afford populations meaningful involvement and human agency. This combination of third-person objectivity and first- person insight arguably adds scientific rigour to the enterprise of global psychology (Stevens & Gielen, 2007): rigour that would probably elude cross-cultural psychologists and indigenous psychologists who work exclusively of one another. Limitations of the indigenous psychology movement
The dilemma was that of wanting IPs to share the prestige of science, while at the same time displaying a reluctance to be shackled by the demands of rigour; it tended to result in more flexible re-definitions of ‘science’. (Jahoda, 2016, p. 177) Summary This chapter tackles claims that the science of human behaviour has long been restricted by a bias towards theories, research methods and publishing interests that are centred in Europe and North America. The proliferation of psychology publishing in selected global locations is examined. Global distribution of psychological practitioners is also examined. Degrees of hegemony in psychological research traditions also come in for scrutiny. We have presented a detailed introduction to critical strands in global research that seek to challenge this hegemony. Critical, community psychology, participatory action research and the indigenous psychology movement are all evidence that psychology is a truly global discipline that can be conducted across, and according to the principles of, diverse life-worlds. The relationship between these critical strands and the mainstream approaches of cross-culturalism remain in transition and disputed. TABLE 5.2 Paradigms of global psychology at a glance
Г REFLECTIVE EXERCISE 16 Match up the definitions on the right with the terms on the left (see p. 207 for answers) ![]() Culture, cognition 6 and intellect |
<< | CONTENTS | >> |
---|
Related topics |