Home Psychology
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Development of epistemic knowledgeIn order to get a more holistic view on adult thinking and reasoning, we also need to have an understanding of the development of epistemic knowledge: how individuals develop conceptions of knowledge and knowing, and how these are utilized in understanding the reality. The research tradition on epistemological beliefs contributes to these issues and is interested in the definition of knowledge; how knowledge is constructed, how knowledge is evaluated, where it resides, and how knowledge occurs (Hofer, 2002). Epistemological beliefs are commonly considered to be the lens through which individuals interpret information, set standards, and decide on an appropriate course of action (Hofer & Pintrich, 2002; see also Limón, 2006). Research references include a wide variety of terms, such as epistemic positions (Perry, 1968), epistemic cognition (King & Kitchener, 2002), epistemological reflection (Baxter Magolda, 1992), epistemological understanding (Kuhn, Cheney, & Weinstock, 2000), and epistemological thinking (Kuhn & Weinstock, 2002). Kaartinen-Koutaniemi and Lindblom-Ylanne (2008) and Seppálá (2013) distinguish two different directions for this tradition: 1) the study of epistemological notions, i.e., related to knowledge, beliefs, and 2) the study of epistemological and reflective thinking. There are numerous classifications in the field, and at least three different approaches for epistemological beliefs and how they are studied: developmental, systematic/structural, and resource point of view (Limón, 2006; see also Moshman, 2013, who distinguishes seven different areas for the study of epistemic cognition, cf. Greene, Tomey-Purta, & Azevedo, 2010). In this chapter two approaches under the research tradition of epistemological beliefs are presented: the developmental approach and the epistemological resources approach. Developmental approachThe developmental approach is widely adopted in epistemological research (Limón, 2006). It focuses on explaining developmental changes in epistemological beliefs and seeks to describe the developmental levels through which an individual progresses. From the historical point of view the most significant name in the field of epistemological development, as well as being a representative of developmental theorists, is William Perry (1968). He was the first to redefine the way cognition develops, by focusing on the development of epistemological assumptions. Perry (1970) examined college students’ paths from adolescence to adulthood by interviewing students over their four years at college. Perry was interested in how the students perceived the world around them. He aimed at “mapping development in the forms of seeing, knowing and caring” (Perry, 1970, p. ix). Perry and his research group conducted interviews at the end of each study year “in as open-ended way as possible” (Perry, 1970, p. 7) to capture the students’ experiences. On the basts of the interview data, Perry' detected nine positions of development, which are explained in Table 3.2. These nine positions of development cover TABLE 3.2 Perry’s (1970) Developmental Scheme. Printed with permission. Copyright The Finnish Educational Research Association
(Continued) TABLE 3.2 (Cont.)
students’ ethical and intellectual development. The development scheme is described in three parts, each comprising three positions. In any of the nine positions the student may suspend or even reverse the process of development. Thus, also delay, deflection, and regression can occur. Perry (1970) differentiated between three forms of deflection. In temporizing the student may pause his or her development in a position, either by exploring the implications of the position in detail or by hesitating to take the next step. In Retreat the student entrenches him- or herself in the dualistic, absolutistic structures of positions 2 and 3. In Escape the student exploits the opportunity for detachment in positions 4 and 5 to avoid the personal responsibility. Perry’s classification of thinking patterns from absolutism to relativism and to commitment has had a strong impact on the research of adult cognitive development and also, often implicitly, even in wisdom research (Barzilai & Eshet-Alkalai, 2015; Kallio, 2015; see Chapter 2). Examples of the later developmental approaches are Kuhn’s (Kuhn et al., 2000; Kuhn & Weinstock, 2002) theory on epistemological thinking, King’s and Kitchener’s (2002) theory of reflective thinking, and Baxter Magolda’s (1992, 2004) model of epistemological reflection. A common element for these developmental models is that the primary task of epistemic development is a progression towards the integration of objectivity and subjectivity. It involves learning to coordinate one’s own subjective perceptions and meaning making with the facts about “objective reality” and the knowledge of authorities (Hofer, 2006). |
<< | CONTENTS | >> |
---|
Related topics |